Differentiating Islamophobia:
Introducing a new scale to measure Islamoprejudice and Secular Islam Critique
Roland Imhoff & Julia Recker
University of Bonn
Accepted for publication in Political Psychology
Keywords: Islamophobia, prejudice, Islam, Muslims, secularism, Islamoprejudice
Author Note
The present research is partly based on a research internship of the second author completed
under the first author’s supervision. The authors would like to thank Aleksandra Lewicki for
helpful comments. Correspondence regarding the article should be addressed to Roland Imhoff,
Department of Psychology, Kaiser-Karl-Ring 9, 53111 Bonn, Germany,
[email protected].
Differentiating Islamophobia 2
Abstract
Since 2001 there has been a steadily increasing awareness of the discrimination against Muslims
based on their religion. Despite the widespread use of the neologism Islamophobia to refer to this
phenomenon, this term has been harshly criticized for confounding prejudiced views of Muslims
with a legitimate critique of Muslim practices based on secular grounds. In the current research a
scale was developed to differentiate Islamoprejudice (based on the influential Islamophobia
definition of the British Runnymede Trust) and Secular Critique of Islam. Across two studies,
Islamoprejudice was related to explicit and implicit prejudice, right-wing authoritarianism and
social dominance orientation whereas Secular Critique was unrelated to any forms of prejudice
but negatively related to religiosity and authoritarianism. The two scales were mostly
independently or only moderately related. Importantly, the new Islamoprejudice scale
outperformed all other scales in predicting actual opposition vs. support for a heatedly debated,
newly built mosque. Implications and application of the differentiation between Islamoprejudice
and Secular Critique will be discussed.
(1173 characters)
Differentiating Islamophobia 3
Since 2001 many populist right-wing parties have gained votes at regional and national
elections throughout Europe with an explicitly endorsed anti-Muslim agenda (e.g., the PVV in
the Netherlands, the FPÖ in Austria, the PRO parties in several German municipalities). In the
United Kingdom, the explicitly anti-Muslim English Defence League (EDL) was founded in
2009 and made the news in 2010 by forging links with the American Tea Party and an
organization called “Stop the Islamization of America” (SIA; see Townsend, 2010). These
developments seem to be rooted in a growing wariness towards Islam. A 2006 poll by the
Washington Post suggested that roughly half of all Americans hold a negative view of Islam
(Deane & Fears, 2006), and particularly concerns about Islamic extremism are shared among the
population majorities in almost all Western countries (Pew Global Attitudes Project, 2006). The
apparent prevalence of wariness and concern about Islam has led several researchers to warn
against a dramatic rise of Islamophobia after the violent attacks on the World Trade Center at the
hands of self-proclaimed Islamic terrorists on September 11th, 2001 (Allen & Nielsen, 2002;
Sheridan, 2006; but see Kaplan, 2006). However, anti-Muslim hostility as a general phenomenon
can be traced further back to the first Gulf war in 1991, the Rushdie affair in 1988, or even the
Islamic revolution in Iran in 1979 (Moodod & Ahmad, 2007; Poynting & Mason, 2007). Across
several historical episodes, the perception of individual Muslims as violent led to an association
of Muslims in general with violence and threat and the perception of Muslim communities in
Western countries as a “fifth column” of a globally threatening aggressive Islam.
Several scholars have subsumed these developments under the neologism Islamophobia
(e.g., Allen, 2007; Gottschalk & Greenberg, 2008). However, despite its widespread use in
political discourse, few concepts have been debated as heatedly over the last ten years as the term
Islamophobia. Best translated as “Fear of Islam”, Islamophobia is considered by some to
currently be the most dangerous form of prejudice, discriminating against individuals on basis of
Differentiating Islamophobia 4
the religious belief in Islam (Allen, 2007; Bunzl, 2005). In 1998, the British non-governmental
organization Runnymede Trust provided a definition of Islamophobia that equated it with closed
(vs. open) views of Islam (Commission on British Muslims and Islamophobia, 1998). According
to this definition, the eight main features of Islamophobia are views of the Islam as (1)
monolithic, (2) separate from and (3) inferior to Western cultures. Islam is seen as (4) “an
enemy” and as (5) a manipulative political ideology. Criticism of the West is (6) a priori rejected,
(7) discrimination against Muslims is justified, and (8) Islamophobia is seen as natural. This
definition remains the most ambitious effort to explicitly define Islamophobia. Use of the term
has become widespread, and yet its definition is highly contested. Two particular criticisms are
regularly raised. One claims that Islamophobia is an expendable neologism that merely describes
a rather well-known phenomenon of prejudice and discrimination against immigrants
(particularly from Muslim countries). The other, more intransigent, objection denounces
Islamophobia as a discursive weapon intended to silence well-justified critique of Islamic
practices and dogmas. We will briefly outline these two critical positions below before
introducing our empirical approach to the question.
Old Wine in a New Bottle
Critics of the term have claimed that Islamophobia is a highly popular, new phrase for a
rather old phenomenon: racism. In this understanding, the new label Islamophobia would conceal
that discrimination and prejudice against immigrants – also but not exclusively from
predominantly Muslim countries – constitute a long-standing tradition in many Western
countries. Thus, Islamophobia is characterized as neologism for racism (Love, 2009; Semati,
2006). Halliday (1999) argued that, in contrast to what the term Islamophobia suggests, “the
enemy is not a faith or a culture but a people” (p. 898). Thus, Salaita (2006) proposed the term
anti-Arab racism as a more accurate replacement for Islamophobia. At the same time, several
Differentiating Islamophobia 5
scholars have argued that religious categories have not only replaced ethnic categories as the
salient part of immigrant self-concepts, but also as political categories (Modood & Ahmad,
2007). However, the term Islamophobia implies that Islam as a religion is the target of
discriminatory practice, when in fact it is individuals who suffer discrimination. One of the most
prominent manifestations of such a view was expressed in the Manifesto signed by twelve
intellectuals and initially published in the French weekly Charlie Hebdo in Feburary 2006 (Hirsi
Ali, Chafiq, Fourest, Levy, Manji, Mozzafari et al., 2006). According to the English translation,
there is no use for the neologism of Islamophobia as it “confuses criticism of Islam as a religion
and stigmatization of those who believe in it”.
Islamophobia as a Discursive Weapon against Enlightened Criticism
Several authors have claimed that allegedly democratically motivated criticism of current
Islamic realities merely serves the same goal as open prejudice: to delegitimize Islam and justify
discrimination against Muslims (Abu Sway, 2006; Fekete, 2004; Semati, 2010). Others have
countered that the use of the term Islamophobia serves specific interests that are beyond
consideration of anti-discrimination. As one of the most outspoken critics of the concept, antiracist organizer Kenan Malik (2005, 2009) provocatively asked whether the hatred and abuse of
Muslims was being exaggerated to silence critics of Islam. A common demand of this position is
that public criticism of aspects of Islam should not be misidentified as prejudiced views, but
rather as enlightened – and secular – critique of religious practices (like veiling, circumcision,
and the ritual slaughtering of animals). The proponents of such criticism claim that “such
practices should be prevented not because they are non-Christian but because they represent a
culture that promotes extreme submission to religion and, hence does not allow individuals to
subscribe to secularist values” (p. 510; Özyürek, 2005). Halliday (1999) warned that the term
Islamophobia also challenged the possibility of dialogue based on universal principles as it
Differentiating Islamophobia 6
suggested “that the solution lies in greater dialogue, bridge-building, respect for the other
community” while running the risk of “denying the right, or possibility, of criticisms of the
practices of those with whom one is having the dialogue” (p. 899). Malik (2005) went as far as
claiming that “the threat of Islamophobia is now being used to stifle criticism of Islam”. Such a
view is also mirrored in the comment of a British senior journalist who characterized
Islamophobia as “a construct that’s been used to cover an awful lot of people and censor debate”
(cited by Meer & Modood, 2009; p. 345). In summary, there exists the fear that Islamophobia is
in essence a derogatory term for legitimate and enlightened critique of certain religious traditions,
habits, and religious regimes.
Considering this wide range of interpretation of what Islamophobia is – from a descriptive
term for hatred directed against Islam to a denunciatory catchword directed against those who
express legitimate criticism of Islam – it seems highly desirable to provide an operational
definition that clearly distinguishes a prejudiced view of Islam from criticism of Islam motivated
by universalistic, secular, and democratic convictions. Such a differentiation is needed in order to
facilitate both research and discussion. We suggest that the neologism Islamophobia may conceal
more than it illuminates and thus developed a scale that incorporates both understandings –
prejudiced and secular views – but clearly differentiates between them. Islamophobia as a
descriptive term of prejudiced or “closed views” (Commission on British Muslims and
Islamophobia, 1998) of Islam is a misnomer when describing prejudice against rather than an
actual fear of Islam (for an Islamophobia definition actually referring to a fear of Islam see Lee,
Gibbons, Thompson, & Timani, 2009). Likewise, other proposals like the term anti-Arab racism
(Salaita, 2006) seem misleading as Arabs are only one small minority in the global community of
Islam (Ummah). Despite general agreement with the definition provided by the Runnymede
Trust, we propose to refer to such prejudiced views of Islam with the term Islamoprejudice rather
Differentiating Islamophobia 7
than Islamophobia. To support the usefulness of this concept it needs to be shown that it is an
internally consistent concept, has any incremental value above and beyond existing prejudice
scales, and that it is not just a denunciatory term for a secular critique of Islam.
The Present Research
To base the political discussion on an empirical ground, we sought criteria to distinguish
prejudiced views of Islam (Islamoprejudice) from secularly motivated criticism of Islam.
Whereas a recently published Islamophobia scale focused on the ‘fear’ aspect of this concept
(Lee et al., 2010) and thus actually holds what the term promises, we sought to align our scale
with a political definition of anti-Islam prejudice as proposed by the Runnymede Trust. Despite
the fact that our definition is based on what the Runnymede Trust calls Islamophobia, we propose
to use the term Islamoprejudice instead. Importantly, we sought to distinguish these prejudiced
views of Islam from secularly motivated critique of certain Islamic practices, thus adding a
measure suitable to tap into non-prejudiced but critical views of Islam.
To this end, a scale was developed and validated in a first study. We constructed a scale
with two hypothesized factors: Islamoprejudice and Secular Criticism of Islam. The items
designed to tap into Islamoprejudice followed directly from the definition of Islamophobia
characterized as closed views of Islam by the Runnymede Trust (Commission on British Muslims
and Islamophobia, 1998). It was hypothesized that the eight closed views of Islam would form a
consistent pattern of thought (which has not been empirically demonstrated thus far).
Furthermore, we expected the Islamoprejudice factor to be highly correlated to established
measures of explicit and implicit prejudice. In contrast, Secular Criticism of Islam was expected
to show no correlation with measures of prejudice. Regarding a locally heatedly debated issue,
we predicted Islamoprejudice to be the strongest predictor of opposition versus support of a
construction of a new representative mosque in Cologne (a larger city close to the location where
Differentiating Islamophobia 8
the sample was recruited). We conducted a first study to empirically test these predictions, then
extracted and cross-validated a shorter version of the scale in Study 2 using a larger and more
diverse sample.
Study 1
In a first study, the validity of the newly created Scale for Islamoprejudice and Secular
Critique of Islam (SIPSCI) was tested by exploring its factorial structure and convergent validity
with explicit prejudice as well as implicit associations between threat and Muslims. The latter
was included as the perception of Muslims as threatening directly and uniquely predicted support
for personal and institutional discrimination of Muslims in a recent study (Doosje, Zimmermann,
Küpper, Zick, & Meertens, 2009). As the study was conducted at a time when the building of a
new representative central mosque in Cologne was being heatedly debated, we included a oneitem measure to assess the support for or opposition against this project. Despite the claim of
opponents that their opposition was merely based on secular concerns, we reasoned that the
position for or against the mosque could be best predicted by Islamoprejudice. Individuals free of
any desire to exclude Muslims from the same rights the majority has should support the mosque,
whereas those high in Islamoprejudice will be opposed to it. Secular Islam Critique was not
expected to be related to this issue, as the denied support would go beyond mere critique of
problematic issues to the level of exclusionary practice.
Method
Participants and Procedure. A sample of 60 students (14 men, 46 women ranging from
19 to 57 years, M = 24.23, SD = 5.92) was recruited in the university building for a study of
person perception and picture categorization. Participants were welcomed and seated in front of a
computer on which they completed all measures. After demographic information and a picture-
Differentiating Islamophobia 9
rating task to uphold the cover story, for one half of the participants the Implicit Association Test
(IAT; Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) was administered, then followed by an irrelevant
distracter questionnaire and the subtle prejudice scale. The other half of the participants received
these three tasks in reverse order. After another picture categorization task, participants
completed the SIPSCI questionnaire and self-categorized either as supportive of the new mosque
or not. They were then thoroughly debriefed and thanked.
Measures.
Islamoprejudice. The Commission on British Muslims and Islamophobia (1998)
proposed eight criteria for a ‘closed view of Islam’, indicative of Islamophobia. For each of these
eight criteria, two or three items were rationally constructed (see Table I for exact wording) to
construct a scale with the following subscales: Islam as an unprogressive monolithic bloc (items
1-3), Islam as separate and other (items 4-5), Islam as inferior (items 6-8), Islam as violent (items
9-10), Islam as mere political ideology (items 11-12), unscreened rejection of any criticism made
by Islam (items 13-14), justification of discriminatory practices towards Muslims (items 15-17),
and acceptance of anti-Muslim hostility (items 18-19). Together, these 19 items (8 items reversecoded, such that high scores indicate low Islamoprejudice) were expected to tap into one
overarching concept of Islamoprejudice.
Secular Critique of Islam. Four criteria were developed to reflect laicist positions
towards Islam regarding the relation of religion and state (rejection of religious authority over
political sphere; items 20-21), gender relations (rejection of gender division; items 22-23), the
adherence to universalist values (rejection of cultural relativism; 24-25), and criticism of Islamic
fundamentalism (items 26-30). A fifth category reflected general secular conviction towards all
religions, not specifically focusing on Islam (items 31-35). These 16 items (3 items reversecoded) were hypothesized to reflect Secular Critique of Islam (see Table I for exact wording).
Differentiating Islamophobia 10
Subtle Prejudice against Muslims and Turks. To explore the relationship between
Islamoprejudice and conventional forms of racism, we included the Subtle Prejudice Scale
(Meertens & Pettigrew, 1997), adapted to Muslims and the largest Muslim group in Germany,
Turks. Each scale has 19 items.
Implicit associations of Muslims with threat. To tap into the implicit associations
between Muslims and threat an Implicit Association Task (IAT; Greenwald et al., 1998) was
constructed. In an IAT, participants have to categorize attribute-relevant items and target
category-relevant items first separately and then in two differently combined blocks
simultaneously. In this IAT, the attribute dimension was “threatening” vs. “non-threatening” and
participants had to categorize words that were pretested to be either clearly non-threatening
(meadow, chick, sunshine, fruit) or threatening (murder, war, terrorist attack, atomic bomb). The
target dimension was “German” vs. “Muslim”. Target items were four pictures in each category:
German targets were faces of Northern European men in front of a neutral background
(restaurant), whereas Muslim targets were faces of Turkish men (the largest Muslim community
in Germany) in front of a mosque. This target dimension was chosen in light of the interpretation
of Islamoprejudice as racist ideology (e.g., Love, 2009) much more than an actual religious
debate (which would have justified using Muslim vs. Christian as target concept dimension and
religious symbols as items).
Support for mosque. The item tapping into support for or opposition against a
representative mosque in nearby Cologne read “Do you support the controversial building of a
new great mosque in Cologne?” (yes/no).
Results
Differentiating Islamophobia 11
An exploratory factor analysis with varimax rotation offered the solution of a two-factor
structure with factors (based on the scree plot) that could be labeled as Islamoprejudice and
Secular Critique of Islam, as per the discussion above. All items related to Islamoprejudice
loaded significantly (>.30) on the first factor and not on the second (with the exception of item
14), and all items hypothesized to reflect Secular Critique loaded on the second factor (>.30).
Consequently, indices were calculated for both subscales that proved highly reliable (Table II)
and uncorrelated (r = .01).
The correlation with the other measures showed that the Islamoprejudice had a strong
overlap with the conventional prejudice measures against Muslims (as a religious group) and
Turks (as a national group), whereas Secular Critique was entirely uncorrelated with these
measures of prejudice. It is conceivable that this pattern emerged merely as a result of differential
social desirability of these scales. It is clearly socially sanctioned to articulate prejudiced views
and the same might hold for the Islamoprejudice scale. Secular critique, in contrast, might be a
more socially accepted form of articulating prejudice by legitimizing it with positive values like
secularity. If that was the case, the indirectly measured implicit association of Muslims with
threat should not show any differential correlation between the two factors. For the IAT, we
calculated a d-score (IAT-d; Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003) that can be interpreted as
individual effect sizes of how much stronger the association between the concepts Muslim and
threat is compared the association between the ingroup (German) and threat. As expected and in
contradiction to the claim of mere social desirability effects, the associative strength between
Muslims and threat (IAT-d) was positively related to Islamoprejudice but not to Secular Critique
of Islam (Table II).
Opposition to the building of a new representative mosque was related to both variants of
the subtle prejudice scale and to implicit association between Muslims and threat as well as the
Differentiating Islamophobia 12
Islamoprejudice subscale, but not to Secular Critique (criterion validity in Table II). As
Islamoprejudice showed remarkably high correlation with established measures of prejudice
against Turks and Muslims, it was crucial to provide evidence for the incremental validity of the
subscale. We thus analyzed its unique relationship to the building of the new central mosque in
Cologne in contrast to subtle prejudice against Turks and Muslims. Almost precisely one half (n
= 31) of the 60 participants claimed to support the building of the new mosque, whereas the other
half (n = 29) did not. Both SIPSCI subscales as well as subtle prejudice against Turk as well as
Muslims were entered together with the IAT score in a binary logistic regression to predict
opposition to (vs. support for) the new mosque. Because the predictors were highly
intercorrelated, a stepwise procedure (forward conditional) was chosen. Results showed that
Islamoprejudice was the strongest predictor, B = 1.36, SE = 0.42, Odds Ratio Exp(B) = 3.89, p =
.001. None of the other variables added any incremental value.
Discussion
The new SIPSCI scale clearly differentiated two different factors: a Secular Critique of
Islam as well as a prejudice-laden closed view of Islam, the latter of which has been referred to as
Islamophobia in the literature but never formally differentiated from the first. Our study is the
first to show that the definition brought forward by the Runnymede Trust (Commission on British
Muslims and Islamophobia, 1998) can be translated into an internally highly consistent set of
individual statements. Correlations with established measures of explicit and implicit prejudice
showed that Islamoprejudice can be best conceptualized as a form of religion-based prejudice that
is linked to the perception of Muslims as threatening. However, importantly the Islamoprejudice
scale outperformed previously existing measures in predicting the position in a concrete political
issue: the building of a new mosque. Debates around the building of new mosques, the height of
Differentiating Islamophobia 13
minarets and other issues concerning the role of Muslims in society have appeared all over
Europe and our results suggest that, although prejudice does play a role in predicting which side
individuals will be take in these debates, the new Islamoprejudice scale is a better predictor than
established measures.
In contrast, the second factor, Secular Critique of Islam, showed no relationship to any of
the prejudice measures. In the case of the explicit prejudice scales, this could be explained by
socially desirable responding. It might have been that some individuals disguise their prejudiced
views as an allegedly enlightened and Secular Critique. However, the lack of a correlation
between the critique scale and automatically activated associations between Muslims and threat
speak against such an interpretation. Instead, it seems that our Secular Critique subscale captured
a relatively consistent pattern of thought that is not contaminated with prejudices but is highly
critical of Islam and religious interferences in the worldly sphere. However, some individuals
might make use of such – not inherently prejudiced – statements to justify their prejudiced views.
As a critical note, it may raise concerns that our factor analysis was based on 35 items but
only 60 participants. This is in contradiction to Cattell (1978), who suggested using a minimum
respondent-to-variable ratio of 3:1. However, more recently MacCallum, Widaman, Zhang, and
Hong (1999) argued that the needed ratio is not the same for every data set. For a dataset with
wide communality and a variable-to-factor-ratio of 20:3, they received 100% convergent
solutions even with a sample size of only 60 in a Monte-Carlo study. As the present dataset also
showed a wide communality (ranging from .10 to .69) and an even better variable-to-factor-ratio
of 35:2, the small sample-to-item ratio may still have produced a robust solution. However, to
confirm these findings, it is advisable to cross-validate our findings. In addition, a shorter (and
thus more easily administered) scale is highly desirable. Thus, for Study 2 the scale length was
Differentiating Islamophobia 14
reduced and the shorter scale was administered to a larger sample to bolster the reliability of our
results.
Study 2
To expand the findings from Study 1 beyond a small student sample, we conducted an
online study with a community sample. The aims were to confirm the dimensionality of the
questionnaire using a short form and to explore its subscales’ relationships to right-wing
authoritarianism (RWA), social dominance orientation (SDO), and religiosity.
Resulting from a view of the world as threatening, right-wing authoritarianism is assumed
to evoke dislike for potentially threatening groups (Altemeyer, 1988, 1998). Islamoprejudice was
thus expected to be positively associated with RWA, as the perception of Muslims as violent,
hostile, and threatening lies at the core of Islamoprejudice. In contrast, Secular Critique should be
negatively related to RWA, because one of the core principles of RWA is authoritarian
submission, i.e. the imperative to show obedience. One application of such obedience concerns
the role of women in society (reflected in the reverse-coded item “The days when women are
submissive should belong strictly in the past. A woman’s place in society should be wherever she
wants to be,” in the RWA scale by Funke, 2005). Along similar lines, the Secular Critique scale
includes statements that oppose the alleged duty of Muslim women to conform to Islamic norms
regarding their position in society.
Another generalized political opinion concerns the tendency to justify existing hierarchies
and derogate low-status groups (Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth, & Malle, 1994). Social dominance
orientation (SDO) describes such a mindset and has been defined as the general tendency to
emphasize one’s own groups’ superiority over other groups (von Collani, 2002). Whereas
conservatism is one of the core components of authoritarianism, SDO contains a firm belief in
Differentiating Islamophobia 15
inequality, independent of any traditionalism. We thus predicted SDO to be positively associated
with Islamoprejudice, as Islamoprejudice may serve as a way to legitimize the ingroup’s
privilege, thus legitimizing an existing hierarchy.
With regard to Secular Critique, the situation may be more complex. On the one hand,
Secular Critique is clearly based on egalitarian norms (e.g., equal rights for women). On the
other hand, it seems unlikely that people who endorse anti-egalitarian beliefs and hold prejudiced
opinions about Muslims (Islamoprejudice) would not also express critique. Voicing an –
allegedly secular – critique might serve as a means to justify existing prejudice and support for
discriminatory practices. In that sense, the critique is still not inherently prejudiced but may serve
as a justification of these anti-egalitarian attitudes. We thus expected participants high in SDO to
show a positive relationship between Islamoprejudice and Secular Critique. The more prejudiced
they are against Muslims, the more they should express criticism of certain Muslim practices as a
form of justification of their prejudice. For low-SDO participants, we expected comparably high
scores on Secular Critique, independent of their (generally lower) degrees of Islamoprejudice.
For religiosity, we expected a dissociation of the relationship to the subscales. The degree
of religiosity for non-Muslims should be positively related to Islamoprejudice, as religious
systems are generally mutually exclusive and this should increase a view of Muslims as different.
However, an opposite prediction was made for the Secular Critique subscale. This scale includes
the rejection of Muslim (but also other) religious intervention into the worldly sphere. Thus,
religious non-Muslims should object to such a Secular Critique as it is potentially applicable to
certain aspects of their own religion as well.
Method
Participants. The study was conducted online and participants were approached via social
network forum and online discussion boards. The data of fifteen participants were discarded as
Differentiating Islamophobia 16
they self-identified as Muslims. The remaining 316 participants (148 men, 168 women) ranged in
age range from 16 to 72 years, M = 29.21, SD = 11.49. The majority of the respondents selfidentified as Christian (106 Christian-Catholic, 116 Christian-Protestant, and 5 ChristianOrthodox), eleven participants belonged to another faith, and the remaining 78 participants
claimed not identify with any specific religion.
Measures.
SIPSCI. A short version of the scale used in Study 1 was created. The criteria for item
selection were high item-total correlations in Study 1 (|r| > .50), a minimum of scale
heterogeneity and a balance of positive and reverse-coded items. The final scale consisted of nine
items tapping into Islamoprejudice and six items tapping into Secular Critique (see Table I for
details).
Perception of Muslims as powerful. A one-item measure tapped into participants’
perception of Muslims as holding a position of power and influence compared to them
(“Compared to myself Muslims have a lot less [a lot more] power and influence” ranging from 1
(a lot less) to 5 (a lot more)).
Generalized political attitudes. We used the German scale by von Collani (2002) to
assess SDO. In addition, RWA was measured with the German scale by Funke (2005).
Religiosity. Participants were asked to indicate the degree to which they perceived
themselves to be religious on a scale ranging from 1 (not religious at all) to 7 (highly religious).
Results
First, confirmatory factor analyses were conducted to test three competing models: a onefactor solution in which all items of the SIPSCI scale were influenced by one latent variable, a
two-factor model with Secular Critique and Islamoprejudice as independent factors, and a two-
Differentiating Islamophobia 17
factor model that allowed for a correlation between Secular Critique and Islamoprejudice. Results
showed that the model with two correlated factors was superior to both alternative models,
particularly the one-factor model (Table III). The data also fit the model with two completely
independent factors, but the fit was not as good. As a result, two separate indices were
calculated; they were both reliable but moderately correlated, r = .21 (Table IV).
We had expected the two scales to be correlated particularly for individuals who endorse
anti-egalitarian attitudes (SDO). To test whether SDO moderated the consistency between the
two subscales, we conducted a moderated regression analysis (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken,
2003). Participants’ Secular Critique scores were regressed on their centered SDO score, their
centered average score on the Islamoprejudice scale, and the cross-product of these two,
representing the interaction term. Results revealed that Islamoprejudice was a positive predictor,
ß = .20, p < .001, and that SDO a marginally negative one, ß = -.13, p = .06. Importantly, this was
qualified by a significant interaction, ß = .17, p = .006 (Figure 1). Simple slope tests at ±1 SD of
SDO fully supported the hypothesis that Islamoprejudice was only a significant predictor of
Secular Critique for high-SDO participants, B = 0.31, SE = 0.07, p < .001, not for low-SDO
participants, B = 0.08, SE = 0.07, p = .26.
To further test for the unique variance associated with the two subscales, we ran regression
analyses in which the shared variance was adjusted for by entering Islamoprejudice and Secular
Critique as simultaneous predictors of SDO, RWA, religiosity, and the perceptions of Muslims as
powerful. Results revealed that Islamoprejudice was related to greater SDO, ß = .49, p < .001,
greater RWA, ß = .66, p < .001, and greater perception of Muslim as powerful, ß = .28, p < .001.
In contrast, Secular Critique was related to less RWA, ß = -.18, p < .001, and less religiosity, ß =
-.14, p = .02. These results fully supported our predictions regarding the dissociation between
Islamoprejudice and Secular Critique.
Differentiating Islamophobia 18
Discussion
The factor structure extracted in Study 1 was replicated with a short and economic version
of the scale (15 items total). Confirmatory factor analyses revealed that models based on the
assumption of two distinct factors showed a superior fit to a one-factor model. Different from
Study 1, the two subscales were not entirely independent, but showed a small correlation. We
have argued and tested that this intercorrelation might due to individual differences in antiegalitarian attitudes. Participants who endorsed such hierarchy-justifying beliefs (SDO) showed a
correlation between Islamoprejudice and Secular Critique, potentially as they make instrumental
use of allegedly unbiased critique to justify their prejudiced opinions.
Study 2 also added empirical evidence for a dissociation of the two subscales. Statistically
adjusting for the shared variance of the subscales, Islamoprejudice was positively related to
authoritarian attitudes (RWA), whereas Secular Critique showed a negative correlation with
RWA. In addition, Secular Critique was negatively related to religiosity in our non-Muslim
sample. This is in line with our conceptualization of Secular Critique as a general opposition to
religious interference in worldly spheres.
General Discussion
Our studies provide the first approach to translate the well-accepted political definition of
Islamophobia proposed by the Runnymede Trust (Commission on British Muslims and
Islamophobia, 1998) into a psychometrically sound scale tapping into individual differences in
Islamoprejudice. Across two samples, the different aspects of Islamoprejudice seem to reflect a
coherent and consistent mindset as indicated highly internally consistent scales. Our results
further show that this definition of Islamophobia (Islamoprejudice) is indeed closely related to
prejudiced opinions about religious (Muslims) and ethnic (Turks) groups. This speaks against the
Differentiating Islamophobia 19
claim that Islamophobia – in the way defined by the Runnymede Trust – is merely a “discursive
weapon” to silence criticism of Islam; it can indeed be characterized as a form of prejudice.
Importantly, however, it was not fully redundant with well-established measures of subtle
prejudice, as it outperformed them in predicting opposition to or support for a public building
project highly associated with opinion about Islam, a representative central mosque. To further
establish construct validity of the newly designed scale, it was shown that Islamoprejudice is
associated with the automatic association of Muslims as threatening and generalized right-wing
political attitudes like RWA and SDO.
In contrast, Secular Critique of Islam (i.e., laicist views that are extremely critical of certain
practices allegedly commanded by Islam, Islamic fundamentalism, and religious authority over
worldly manners) was unrelated to explicit and implicit prejudice. Thus, potentially harsh
criticism of certain Islamic practices should not be mistakenly confused with Islamoprejudice.
Although our results from Study 2 show that some (highly anti-egalitarian) individuals may
express criticism only to the degree that they are prejudiced, our scale seems to tap into
universalistic concerns about repressive aspects of Islam.
Our scale provides a chance to distinguish these two facets of opinions about Islam that are
often confused under the overarching label of Islamophobia when in fact they are very distinct.
Importantly, we have relied on the so far most ambitious effort to define closed views of Islam by
the Runnymede Trust but suggested using a different label than Islamophobia. Islamophobia
implies that it is predominantly a fear of Islam that stands behind these closed views when in fact
there is little empirical support for such a claim. Lee and colleagues (2009) have recently
presented an Islamophobia scale that covers specifically fearful reactions to Islam. Their scale
does not examine the construct defined by the Runnymede Trust, a gap filled by our
Islamoprejudice subscale. However, we further propose to clarify the debate about Islamophobia
Differentiating Islamophobia 20
by strictly differentiating between a negative and closed view of Islam (Islamoprejudice, our
subscale), predominantly fearful reaction to Islam (Lee et al., 2009), and non-prejudiced but
highly critical views of Islam (Secular Critique, our scale).
Outlook
Future research might benefit from the distinction established in the SIPSCI to further
elucidate whether political stands in current debates are motivated by authentic concerns about
democratic values or are rather an expression of a rise in prejudices against Muslims. Political
campaigning against the rising influence of populist parties across the Western world might
differentially target voters motivated by Secular Critique and those whose prejudiced views led
them to vote for parties like the Dutch PPV (Partij voor de vrijheid), the Austrian FPÖ
(Freiheitliche Partei Österreichs), the French FN (Front National), the German NPD
(Nationaldemokratische Partei Deutschlands), or the British EDL (English Defence League), or
the US-based SIA (Stop the Islamization of Amercia).
Conclusion
Our research has shown the existence of a rather consistent pattern of opinions about Islam
that can be called prejudiced. At the same time, not just any form of admittedly harsh criticism of
Islam needs to be associated with explicit or even automatically activated prejudices. Our results
support the previous criticism that Islamophobia may be an expendable neologism for a
phenomenon already known for long: racist prejudice (e.g., Love, 2009; Semati, 2006). In fact,
our data suggest that Islamoprejudice as we propose to call it accordingly is highly related to
prejudice against Muslims in general but also to ethnic groups like Turks specifically
(independent of the fact that a majority of Turks might consider themselves as non-religious).
However, we have also provided empirical support for the fact that Islamoprejudice is a suitable
concept to make very specific predictions about political standpoints on Islam-related issues, like
Differentiating Islamophobia 21
the building of mosques. We have also found support for the notion that it is ill-advised to
denunciate potentially harsh – but secularly motivated – criticism of Islamic practices as
inherently prejudiced. Thus, the implications of our results go beyond the mere introduction of a
new scale.
Differentiating Islamophobia 22
References
Abu Sway, M. (2006). Islamophobia: Meaning, Manifestation, Causes. In H. Schenker & Z. AbuZayyad (Eds.) Islamophobia and Anti-Semitism (pp. 13-23). Princeton: Markus Wiener.
Allen, C. (2007). Islamophobia and its consequences. In S. Amghar, A. Boubekeur, & M.
Emerson (Eds.) European Islam – Challenges for public policy and society (pp. 144-167).
Brussels: Center for European Policy Studies.
Allen, C., & Nielsen, J. S. (2002). Summary Report on Islamophobia in the EU15 after 11
September 2001. Vienna: European Monitoring Centre for Racism and Xenophobia.
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of freedom: Understanding right-wing authoritarianism. San
Francisco: Jossey Bass.
Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other “authoritarian personality”. Advances in Experimental Social
Psychology, 30, 47-92.
Bunzl, M. (2005). Between anti-Semitism and Islamophobia: Some thoughts on the new Europe.
American Ethnologist, 32, 499-508.
Cattell, R. B. (1978). The Scientific Use of Factor Analysis. New York: Plenum.
Commission on British Muslims and Islamophobia. (1997). Islamophobia: A challenge for us all.
London: Runnymede Trust.
Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple regression/
correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd edition). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Deane, C., & Fears, D. (2006, March 9). Negative perception of Islam increasing. Washington
Post, p. A01. Retrieved November 12, 2010 from http://www.washingtonpost.com/wpdyn/content/article/2006/03/08/AR2006030802221_pf.html.
Doosje, B., Zimmermann, A., Küpper, B., Zick, A. & Meertens, R. (2009). Terrorist threat and
perceived Islamic support for terrorist attacks as predictors of personal and institutional out-
Differentiating Islamophobia 23
group discrimination and support for anti-immigration policies – evidence from 9 European
countries. Revue Internationale de Psychologie Sociale, 22, 203-233.
Fekete, L. (2004). Anti-Muslim racism and the European security state. Race & Class, 46, 3-29.
Funke, F. (2005). The dimensionality of right-wing authoritarianism: Lessons from the dilemma
between theory and measurement. Political Psychology, 26, 195-218.
Gottschalk, P., & Greenberg, G. (2008). Islamophobia: Making Muslims the enemy. Lanham:
Rowan & Littlefield.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. K. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences
in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 74, 1464-1480.
Greenwald, A. G, Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit
Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 85, 197-216.
Halliday, F. (1999). ‘Islamophobia’ reconsidered. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 22, 892-902.
Hirsi Ali, A., Chafiq, C., Fourest, C, Levy, B.-H., Manji, I., Mozzafaro, M. et al. (2006, March
1). Manifesto: Together facing the new totalitarianism. Retrieved on November 12, 2010,
from http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/europe/4764730.stm.
Kaplan, J. (2006). Islamophobia in America? September 11 and Islamophobic hat crime.
Terrorism and Political Violence, 18, 1-33.
Lee, S. A., Gibbons, J. A., Thompson, J. M., & Timani, H. S. (2009). The Islamophobia scale:
Instrument development and initial validation. The International Journal for the
Psychology of Religion, 19, 92-105.
Love, E. (2009). Confronting Islamophobia in the United States: framing civil rights activism
among Middle Eastern Americans. Patterns of Prejudice, 43, 401-425.
Differentiating Islamophobia 24
MacCallum, R. C., Widaman, K. F., Zhang, S., & Hong, S. (1999). Sample size in factor analysis.
Psychological Methods, 4, 84-99.
Malik, K. (2005, January 7). What hate? The Guardian. Retrieved November 17, 2010, from
http://www.guardian.co.uk/world/2005/jan/07/religion.islam/print.
Malik, K. (2009). From Fatwa to Jihad: The Rushdie Affair and its Legacy. London: Atlantic.
Meer, N. & Modood, T. (2009). Refutations of racism in the ‘Muslim question’. Patterns of
prejudice, 43, 335-354.
Meertens, R. W., & Pettigrew, T. F. (1997). Is subtle Prejudice really Prejudice? Public Opinion
Quarterly, 61, 54-71.
Modood, T., & Ahmad, F. (2007). British Muslim perspectives on multiculturalism. Theory,
Culture & Society, 24, 187-213.
Özyürek, E. (2005). The politics of cultural unification, secularism, and the place of Islam in the
new Europe. American Ethnologist, 32, 509-512.
Pew Global Attitudes Project (2006, June). The great divide: How Westerners and Muslims view
each other. Washington: Pew Research Center. Retrieved November 13, 2010 from
http://pewglobal.org/2006/06/22/the-great-divide-how-westerners-and-muslims-view-eachother/.
Poynting, S., & Mason, V. (2007). The resistible rise of Islamophobia: Anti-Muslim racism in the
UK and Australia before 11 September 2001. Journal of Sociology, 43, 61-86.
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social Dominance Orientation:
A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 67, 741-763.
Salaita, S. (2006). Beyond orientalism and Islamophobia – 9/11, anti-Arab racism, and the
mythos of national pride. Cr- the New Centennial Review, 6, 245-266.
Differentiating Islamophobia 25
Semati, M. (2010). Islamophobia, culture and race in the age of empire. Cultural Studies, 24,
256-275.
Sheridan, L. P. (2006). Islamophobia Pre- and Post-September 11th, 2001. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 21, 317-336.
Townsend, M. (2010, October 10). English Defence League forges links with America's Tea
Party. The Observer, Retrieved November 17, 2010, from
http://www.guardian.co.uk/uk/2010/oct/10/english-defence-league-tea-party.
von Collani, G. (2002). Das Konstrukt der Sozialen Dominanzorientierung als generalisierte
Einstellung: eine Replikation [The construct of Social Dominance Orientation as a
generalized attitude: a replication]. Zeitschrift für Politische Psychologie, 10, 263-282.
Differentiating Islamophobia 26
Table I
Items and Corresponding Factor Loadings of the SIPSCI in Studies 1 and 2
Study 1
Study 2
1
2
1
2
1.
The Islamic world is backward and unresponsive to new realities.
.54
.20
-
-
2.
I think it is wrong to characterize the Islamic world as a uniform
-.53
.25
-.54
.25
-.40
.06
-
-
.75
.05
.75
.09
-.63
.07
formation.
3.
It is wrong to lump together all Muslims.
4.
Muslim cultures have so fundamentally different values, that it is
difficult to identify common aims or ideals.
5.
Islam and Christianity share the same universal ethical principles.
-.70 -.02
6.
Islam is an archaic religion, unable to adjust to the present.
.80
.22
.80
.17
7.
Compared to West Europeans Muslims are rather irrational.
.63
.23
.79
.18
8.
Compared to other religious and philosophical approaches Islam is
.63 -.01
.79
.05
-.34 -.11
-
-
rather primitive.
9.
I don’t think it is justified to speak of a clash of the culture between
Islam and the West.
10. I think Islamic religion and its aggressive sides predispose it
.67
.18
.79
.10
-.37
.08
-
-
.34 -.12
-
-
.50 -.03
-
-
.41
-
-
.53 -.19
-
-
.68 -.10
.67
.03
towards proximity to terrorism.
11. I think Islam is above all a religion as therefore has nothing to do
with politics or war.
12. Islam is a political ideology used to serve political and military
goals.
13. Any critique of the West brought forward by representatives of
Islam is exaggerated und unjustified.
14. When Muslims criticize certain aspects of our Western life style we
-.32
should at least listen to their points.
15. It is completely legitimate to exclude Muslims from certain key
positions as our society is essentially Christian.
16. Muslims and their religion are so different from us that it would be
naive to demand an equal access to all positions in society.
17. Wherever a large number a Muslims live and visit the schools,
-.71
.11
.52
.11
-.49 -.11
Islamic religious education should be offered.
18. Today it is not uncommon to be suspicious of Muslims.
-
-
Differentiating Islamophobia 27
19. Hostility against Muslims is an intolerable form of discrimination.
-.41
.27
-
-
20. The strict division of church and state is a Western accomplishment
.22
.72
.03
.75
-.01
.62
-
-
.23
.63
.21
.55
.02
.61
.17
.55
.00 -.40
-
-
that would be a progress in many Islamic shaped countries.
21. It is a scandal that in some countries noncompliance with religious
rules of the Islam results in earthly punishment.
22. Although some women voluntarily wear a veil one should not
ignore that for some women it also means coercion.
23. The rigid Islamic gender division should not be conceded to –
neither in public health sector nor in physical education.
24. The criticism of so-called fundamentalists only fuels the baiting
against all Muslims and should be labeled racism.
25. The term Islamophobia can be misused to make all Muslims feel
-.03
.37
-
-
.29
-.17
.58
.67
.31
-
.44
-
-.15
.37
-
-
.15 -.40
.03 .37
-
-
like victims and thus prevent a necessary conflict between
moderate and fundamentalist fractions of the community.
26. It is wrong to ignore the threat by fundamentalist Islam.
27. We should support those liberal Muslims who distance from
fundamentalist interpretations of Islam.
28. One can fight against the political ideology of Islamic
fundamentalism without having anything against nonfundamentalist Muslims.
29. Islamic fundamentalists fight for a legitimate cause.
30. Islam is above all a religion, but fundamentalist Islam is a political
ideology.
31. Religion should be private matter, to use one’s own religious
-.09
.50
-
-
-.12
.61
-
-
.08
.68
-.14
.55
.03 -.32
-
-
-.08
.76
convictions and feelings as a measure of how others should behave
is undemocratic.
32. Universal human rights and certain legal norms should always
stand above religious rules.
33. Religion becomes a problem when humans try to take the holy
writing literally.
34. I think that religion – no matter which one – mobilizes the best
within humans.
35. As any other religion one must criticize Islam and its representative
when they interfere with non-religious issues.
.07
.51
Table II
Descriptives and intercorrelations for all continuous measures in Study 1.
Criterion Validity
Descriptives
Correlations
Opposition to Mosque
α
M
SD
1.
1. Islamoprejudice
.87
3.18
0.84
-
2. Secular Critique of Islam
.82
5.68
0.70
.01
3. IAT-d
.81
0.66
0.34
.47**
.08
-
4. Subtle Prejudice Muslims
.80
3.75
0.51
.72**
-.02
.34**
-
.69*
5. Subtle Prejudice Turks
.82
3.63
0.51
.66**
-.04
.29**
.85**
.65*
2.
3.
4.
AUC
.76**
.72**
Note. All scales (except IAT-d) ranging from 1 to 7. The IAT-d score is an individual effect size. Greater scores represent
stronger association between Islam and threat.
Differentiating Islamophobia 29
Table III
Fit indices for three different models in confirmatory factor analysis (CFA)
χ2
χ2/df
222.02***
2.49
Model
1. Two correlated factors
Difference between Model 1 and Model 2
2. Two independent factors
CI 90%
AIC
RMSEA
(PCLOSE)
0.07
0.06 – 0.08 (**)
314.02
0.07
0.06 – 0.08 (**)
321.14
0.11
0.10 – 0.12 (***)
498.07
9.12*
231.14***
2.57
Difference between Model 2 and Model 3
3. One factor
Δχ2
176.93***
408.07***
Note: N = 316; *** p < .001, ** p < .01, * p < .05
4.53
Differentiating Islamophobia 30
Table IV
Descriptives and intercorrelations for all continuous measures in Study 2.
Descriptives
Correlations
α
M
SD
1.
1. Islamoprejudice
.88
3.27
1.34
-
2. Secular Critique of Islam
.71
5.79
0.89
.21**
-
-
2.49
0.88
.26**
-.01
-
4. RWA
.81
2.97
0.98
.62**
-.05
.20**
5. SDO
.91
2.56
1.03
.49**
.06
.07
.54**
-
-
1.95
1.00
.06
-.12*
.09
.22**
-.05
3. Powerful Muslims
6. Religiosity
2.
3.
4.
5.
-
Note. All multiple-item scales (1, 2, 4, 5) ranging from 1 to 7. Perceptions of Muslims as powerful (3) ranging from 1
(much less power and influence than me) to 5 (much more power and influence than me), religiosity (6) ranging from 1
(not religious at all) to 4 (very religious). N = 316.
Figure Captions
Figure 1. Secular Critique of Islam as a function of the interplay between Social Dominance
Orientation (SDO) and Islamoprejudice plotted at ±1SD.
Differentiating Islamophobia 32
Figure 1
Secular Critique of Islam
6,5
6,0
Low SDO (-1SD)
High SDO (+1SD)
5,5
5,0
0,0
-1 SD
+1 SD
Islamoprejudice